Politics
Peace 2025
 
Sexology
Text styles
 
Ethics
Music
Contact
     
Ion Mittler:  Look at the human
                                     
Q09B   Man’s body and clothing
Content warning:
This report contains statistical information about the erotic interests of adult people towards men and boys, who are dressed either very scantily or even quite ordinarily, which might feel shocking for some readers.
This survey was done 8 – 12 january 2023, with the title ”The styles of a man’s clothing: what is HOT, and what is NOT?” Photos of 72 different styles of men’s clothing were presented to anonymous respondents, ranging from a bedouin’s desert cloak, which covers nearly everything, all the way to the scantiest beachwear.
The respondents were asked for an opinion, how strongly erotic each style of men’s clothing is for them, on the scale 0 (fully neutral and unerotic) – 10 (extremely stimulating sexually, even comparable to pornography). People were also asked for a perception of the eroticness of nudity, but no photos were presented of this level of clothing, unlike in the other alternatives. There were respondents both from Finland and abroad, mostly from western countries. The survey was done with the help of a website that was specifically designed for this purpose.
This report lists the questions about men’s clothing in the order of the total scores that were given by 24 heterosexual women, from the most erotic to the least erotic. A different summary of the results and conclusions of this survey is in chapter Q09C of the book. This report mentions also the answers given by three homosexual men, as an example of what kinds of feelings a man’s scanty clothing can inspire in homosexual men.
The previous survey Q09A examined the attitude of heterosexual men toward women’s body and clothing, which was mostly quite logical and straightforward: the scantier a woman’s clothing is, the higher points men gave to women for eroticness.
The interest of heterosexual women toward men does not follow similar logicality in this survey. Women did not give highest points to men who are dressed as scantily as possible, but rather to men who were often dressed very modestly and loosely. Most women deemed it uninteresting or even disturbing, and gave emphatically low points for eroticness, if a man’s clothing is revealing or skin-tight at the lower body. Women tolerated scanty clothing on men’s upper body better, and the case GA, which received the highest score, contains muscular men without shirt.
What was said above applies to the average of the answers of all women, but some female individuals have given high points also to men who are dressed with notable scantiness. Women’s attitude toward a nude man seems to be distributed evenly on various alternatives, so that the probability of each score 0 – 10 is nearly equally high. On the contrary, the scores given by heterosexual men are concentrated on the highest points, if the woman is scantily dressed or nude.
8.75 eroticness of a nude
woman, for hetero-
sexual men (estimation)
5.00 eroticness of a nude
man, for heterosexual
women (estimation)
These two diagrams give a general overview of how greatly women’s attitude toward men’s nudity differs statistically from men’s attitude toward women’s nudity. And on the other hand, how great variation exists among heterosexual women regarding one’s attitude toward men’s nudity or scanty clothing. Indeed as great variation as is theoretically possible.
It is approximately equally probable that a heterosexual woman has a completely nonchalant and neutral attitude (0 points) toward the nudity of men (who are romantically irrelevant for her), or that she has a very strong erotic interest in it (10 points). In comparison, some 90 % of heterosexual men have a very strong or quite strong erotic interest in women’s nudity (7 – 10 points).
The scores given by women do not follow similar logics as the scores given by men. What kind of logics do they follow, then? The male body as an erotic object seems to be in a quite small role in women’s answers: too revealing clothing of a man is deemed even disturbing, especially what comes to the lower body. Instead, the attractiveness of a man’s face and the general stylishness of the person (maybe also from a romantic point of view) might be emphasized as grounds for the points that women give. The scores given by women do not correlate logically and straightforwardly with how revealing a man’s clothing is. Therefore, for most women the grounds for giving high points seem to be something completely else than the aspect of “decent” clothing, which the survey attempts to measure. Such as stylishness, romantic attractiveness or the sense of safety. Many of the women seem to deem a male stranger, who is scantily dressed on the lower body, to be unpleasant or perhaps even threatening.
A man’s outward appearance seems nevertheless to have great significance for the points that women give. Women have appreciated men’s stylish faces, which can preferably have some stubble of beard. They have also appreciated masculinity and abundant skin hair, preferably combined with athletic muscularity. Muscular men without masculine abundance of skin hair have not interested much the women who answered the survey, if the skin hair has been shaven off.
Pic 1, GA, 94 %. Pic 2, GB, 92 %. Pic 3, GC, 91 %.
GA
4.69
no shirt, upper body from
front, beard, muscular, no
bodybuilder, chest hair
GB
4.59
upper body front, sleeve
of shirt to elbow, some
beard, hairy forearms
GC
4.56
upper body, front and
behind, sporty, shorts,
no shirt, no chest hair
GD
4.44
body from front and be-
hind, jeans and shirt with
a bit scanty looseness
GE
4.29
upper body from behind,
muscular back, but
not a bodybuilder
GF
4.12
upper body front, no
beard, business suit or
a shirt with long sleeves
GG
4.11
upper body from front,
shirt with a bit
scanty looseness
GH
3.94
body from front and be-
hind, loose clothes, jeans
and shirt, or business suit
These eight outward appearances of men, which received the highest scores, indicate that the erotic interest of heterosexual women is not straightforwardly directed at men whose clothing is as scanty as possible. Case GB, which was deemed to be the second most erotic, has a man’s upper body in a loose T-shirt. Ranks 4, 6 and 8 include even less revealing and quite loose clothing of men. Small thongs, which cover only the genitals of a man’s body (case HG), are found at a rank as low as 31 in this list that contains 72 styles of clothing. Bare skin or skin-tight clothing at the pelvis region seems to function as a quite effective repellant of high scores.
Under the photos is indicated the percentage of the score that the style of clothing received, compared to the assumed eroticness 5.00 points of a fully nude man. For example, the score 4.69 of case GA is 94 % of the reference value 5.00. In chapter Q09C of the book I explain, on what grounds I use 5.00 as the assumed score, which the respondents probably would have given to fully nude men, if such photos had been shown to them.
Pic 4, GD*, 89 %. Pic 5, GE, 86 %. Pic 6, GG, 82 %.
* Picture 4 is not a photo that was used in the question GD, but rather a sample picture that has a similar style.
The highest score 4.69 given by heterosexual women is 59 % of the highest score 7.97 given by heterosexual men. The survey gives such an impression that the scores given by these two groups of people are comparable as such, without a mathematical correction factor. Heterosexual women gave 41 % lower scores than heterosexual men to scanty clothing of the opposite gender, probably because their erotic interest toward the male body is 41 % less enthusiastic than the interest of hetero men toward the female body.
The face and the upper body interested hetero women most in the male body, either with or without a shirt on. A man’s upper body may even be ”objectified” with high quality requirements, so that muscularity and skin hair bring many additional points for eroticness.
Case GA, which received the highest score from women, is part of a set of five questions, which displayed a man’s upper body without shirt, so that the man’s body is either very ordinary or emphatically muscular (but not a bodybuilder), and the man has either a hairy chest or a completely clean-shaven upper body.
Of this set of five questions, the highest score went to a combination of musciularity and hairy chest in the case GA (4.69). The next most popular three options received much lower scores, but quite evenly: an ordinary man’s upper body without muscularity, clean-shaven, got 3.00 points (GM), an ordinary upper body with hairy chest 2.89 points (GO), and a clean-shaven emphatically muscular upper body received 2.78 points (GP).
The fifth alternative got a blunt review from women, only 0.59 points to the case KC, which included 18-year-old men with a slim and teenageish upper body, without any trace of muscularity. The women who ansered the survey have clearly expected a man’s body to have emphatically adultlike and preferably also muscular masculinity. The age of the respondents was not asked, but it can be assumed that their average age is probably around the median age of active adult population, maybe approximately 45 years. The case KC might have received higher points from womend who are under 20 years of age.
Homosexual men appreciated most the muscular men with a hairy chest (GA, 5.67) among these five aforementioned cases. 18-year-old teenageish slim men received 4.50 points (KC), and shirtless men with an ordinary body got 4.00 points (GO). In the two cases that received lowest points, the sample size is one respondent only: men with an ordinary body and clean-shaven upper body 3.00 points (GM), and muscular men with a clean-shaven upper body 1.00 points (GP).
At the end of this report is discussed also case YC, in which the upper body of an athletic boy approximately 9 years of age received 3.29 points from women, and 8.50 points from homosexual men, which is the highest score among all 72 questions. (The photo had been cropped in such a way that it was difficult for the respondents to estimate the age of the person that is seen in the picture.) From this we can draw the conclusion that the reason for women’s low interest toward case KC is more due to lack of muscularity than due to the youthfulness of the person’s outward appearance.
Pic 7, GF (4.12) 82 %. Pic 8, GH (3.94) 79 %.
Decent clothing seems to be a concept that is quite difficult to define for a man, at least if the intention were to define such a level of clothing, which would ensure that the man’s outward appearance is not statistically “too strongly erotic” in the opinion of heterosexual women. Perhaps a “too erotic” level of men’s clothing does not even exist, if none of the styles of men’s clothing receive from women a score that would reach even the middle of the given scale (5.00). Unstylish, disturbing or reproachable styles of clothing seem to exist, however.
For homosexual men the situation is very different, if something can be concluded based on a sample of three respondents only. As an average, they gave twice higher points for eroticness to men’s clothing styles than heterosexual women, and high scores were given also to styles of clothing that veiled the hips region and the genitals in an emphatically scanty manner. Precisely such styles, which turned out to be a reason for heterosexual women to leave the highest points ungiven.
There is an urban myth that a woman’s body is erotic and interesting, but a man’s body is not very interesting nor erotic. Based on this survey, the truth seems to be that the eroticness and attractiveness of a person’s body depends on the gender of the watcher, not the gender of the body that is being watched. Most men regard the stylish bodies of other people as erotic and interesting (while taking into account one’s sexual orientation), but most women do not regard the bodies of other people as very interesting “objects”.
GI
3.75
muscular, sleeveless
shirt, upper body only,
from side and diagonally
In the question GI muscular men in a sleevess shirt received 3.75 points for eroticness from heterosexual women. Sporty but less muscular men in a sleeveless shirt got 2.50 points in the case GT. The answers of women indicate an interest in a man’s muscularity. Men with an ordinary body have also received high points e.g. in the case GB (4.59) and GK (3.29), in which they had abundant skin hair on the arms.
In comparison, heterosexual men gave 4.28 points for women’s sleevess clothing in the question BY.
Pic 9, GI, 75 %. Pic 10, GJ, 72 %. Pic 11, GM, 60 %.
GJ
3.61
leggings or underpants to
half-thigh, front & behind,
muscular or ordinary
Question GJ, ranked 10th, is the only one among the 16 cases with highest points, in which the region of genitals is presented in skin-tight clothing, and the first case that does not display the man’s upper body at all.
This question showed the front side of such underpants reaching to mid-thigh, which do not cut the angles short at the genitals, but rather form a bag around the penis. This has not repelled women from giving quite high points. (3.61 would not be a high score for any other group of people, but for hetero women this is a higher than average score.) All other questions, which display skin-tight clothing at the region of hips or genitals, are found much lower in the list.
GK
3.29
upper body front, shirt
sleeve to mid-biceps,
hairiness, not muscular
GL
3.11
man’s fingers
and palms of hand,
in close-up picture
Pic 12, GK (3.29) 66 %. Pic 13, GL (3.11) 62 %.
In the question GL women gave 3.11 points for eroticness to men’s palms of hands and fingers. Homosexual men gave 5.00 points, with a sample of three respondents. Hetero men gave 2.71 points to women’s palms of hands and fingers without nail polish (CK), and 3.41 points with artificial nails (CD).
GM
3.00
upper body front,
no shirt, sporty,
clean-shaven skin
GN
2.94
lower body only, front
and behind, loose
jeans or suit trousers
GO
2.89
upper body front, no
shirt, not muscular,
some chest hair
GP
2.78
upper body front,
no shirt, muscular,
clean-shaven skin
Pic 14, GN, 59 %. Pic 15, GO, 58 %. Pic 16, GP, 56 %.
In the case GN women gave 2.94 points to photos that showed a man’s lower body only, from the waist down, in quite loose trousers from the front and behind. Homosexual men gave as high points as 5.50, with a sample of two respondents, who were the two persons that had the habit of giving highest scores generally. Heterosexual men gave 1.84 points to a woman’s lower body in very loose trousers, in the case CR.
Heterosexual women gave the lower points for eroticness to men’s lower body, the skin-tighter were the trousers that the man was wearing: 2.94 points to very loose trousers in the question GN, 2.67 points to trousers with a bit scanty looseness in the question GQ, and 2.56 points to very tight jeans in the case GS. The difference between the scores of these questions is very small, however.
On the contrary, heterosexual men gave the higher scores for eroticness to women’s trousers, the less they had looseness: 1.84 points to very loose trousers (question CR), 4.06 points to trousers with a bit scanty looseness (CA), and as much as 6.60 points to tight jeans that conform to the shape of the buttocks.
GQ
2.67
trousers with a bit scanty
looseness, lower body
only, front and behind
GR
2.59
shorts to mid-thigh, pics
of the lower body, and
one full body sitting
GS
2.56
tight jeans / leggings,
penis forms a clear
bulge at the crotch
Pic 17, GQ, 53 %. Pic 18, GR, 52 %. Pic 19, GS, 51 %.
Also the scores given by homosexual men were often the higher, the tighter and more closely conforming to the skin were the trousers on a man. The small number of respondents can make the results unreliable, and perhaps unstable logically. Homosexual men gave 8.00 points to skin-tight trousers in the case GS, 7.00 points to trousers wth a bit scanty looseness in the question GQ, and 5.50 points to very loose trousers in the case GN.
Question GS differs from women’s general policy in the aspect that in this case women accepted clearly provocative body postures of a man with at least quite high points (2.56 is 55 % of the highest score 4.69 given by women). As “provocative” body postures have been assumed e.g. crawling on all fours, or with the legs spread quite wide either sitting, lying down or on the knees. (Such body postures have not been used in the primary questions for the styles of clothing, so that the questions would measure the eroticness of the style of clothing, not the eroticness of the body posture.)
Heterosexual women gave low scores to provocative postures of a man’s body: 2.56 (GS), 1.28 (HJ), 1.00 (HP), 0.82 (HS) and 0.76 (HU). Homosexual men were much more interested in such body postures of men, giving points 8.00 (GS), 6.33 (HS), 6.00 (HP), 4.67 (HJ) and 4.00 (HU). Also hetero men gave high scores to somewhat provocative postures of a woman’s body: 7.97 (AA), 7.81 (AB), 7.50 (AC), 6.94 (AK), 6.77 (AM), 6.63 (AQ), 6.06 (BA) and 5.97 (BB).
GT
2.50
sleeveless loose
shirt, front side
only, not muscular
GU
2.41
nipples are seen as a
three-dimensional form
through a bit scanty shirt
In the question GU (2.41 points) women showed quite unimpressive interest to the detail that a man’s nipples are seen through the shirt as a three-dimensional form. Women gave much higher scores to quite similar, a bit scanty shirts in the question GG (4.11), in which the man’s nipples are not seen through the shirt. The title of question GU mentioned that the nipples are seen through the shirt in the photos, to ensure that this aspect will not go unnoticed by the respondents.
Heterosexual men had a much more enthusiastic attitude toward women’s nipples, giving as many as 7.45 points to a skin-tight shirt without brassiere in the case AF, when the nipples and the shape of breasts are seen through the shirt. And 5.76 points in the case BG, in which nipples are not seen through a skin-tight shirt. Thus the men raised the score by nearly 2.00 points, when a woman’s shirt reveals the nipples and the true shape of breasts as a three-dimensional form, without the modifying effect of brassiere.
Pic 20, GT (2.50) 50 %. Pic 21, GU (2.41) 48 %.
Also homosexual men gave high scores to men’s nipples that are seen through the shirt: 7.50 points with a sample of two respondents. (This book includes a statistical research, according to which it is very common that a man’s nipples are seen through a T-shirt. The statistical probability was found to be 41 %, when observing the first 105 men passing by on a summery street, who were wearing a short-sleeved shirt. Women do not seem to be much interested in this aspect, but homosexual men might possibly be indeed.)
GV
2.31
without shirt, from
behind only, bare
back, not muscular
In the question GV women gave 2.31 points for eroticness to such men’s upper bodies, seen from the backside, which were sporty but not muscular. In the case GE women gave nearly twice more points (4.29) to the backs of muscular men.
Homosexual men appreciated a muscular back with 6.00 points, and a back with an ordinary body with 4.00 points. The sample size is very small, however, and the difference between the scores of these two questions can be explained by the fact that the person who generally gave low points did not answer the question about a muscular back.
Pic 5, GE, 86 %. Pic 22, GV, 46 %. Pic 23, GW, 44 %.
GW
2.18
lower body only, standing,
leggings, not muscular,
seen from behind only
Case GW used the same photo of a homosexual man in leggings seen from behind, which was in the misleading case MA in the survey on women’s clothing. Women gave 2.18 points for eroticness, which matches the general policy that women give low scores for skin-tight clothing on a man’s lower body.
Heterosexual men gave 6.16 points for eroticness to the same photo in the misleading case MA (in which the respondents thought the person in the photo to be a woman). This is on par with the scores that men gave to women who wear leggings. Homosexual men gave 5.50 points to this photo, with a sample of two respondents.
GX
2.17
skin-tight shirt,
muscular or ordinary,
or a bit overweight
Case GX had four photos of men’s upper body, clothed in a tight shirt that conforms to the body. This case received the highest score among such questions, in which men are wearing a completely skin-tight shirt. The difference in looseness of shirt is quite small compared to some of the earlier questions, though.
Women gave lower points than might have been expected, considering that 4.11 points were given in the case GG to only a bit looser shirts, and the case GA that received the highest score (4.69) has men without shirt. Perhaps women do not regard a man’s skin-tight shirt as stylish, or the respondents did not regard precisely those men as attractive, who were in the photos of the question GX. Some of them did not have a sporty body.
Pic 24, GX, 43 %. Pic 25, GY, 39 %. Pic 26, GZ, 39 %.
GY
1.94
in swimpants, front, body
from knees upwards, dia-
gonally cut lower edge
GZ
1.94
in swimpants, front, body
from the ankles upwards,
horizontal lower edge
Questions GY and GZ have quite young and sporty men in ordinary skin-tight swimming pants, and the body is seen from the front side only, from below the knees upwards. In one of these questions the lower edge of the swimming pants has a nearly horizontal cut, and in the other question the cut of swimpants is diagonal at an approximately 45 degrees angle, parallel with the groin crease.
Such a difference in the bottom edge cut of the swimming pants does not seem to affect the scores given by women. One of the men in the question GZ is quite muscular, with hairiness on the upper body, so his appearance does not differ much from question GA, which received the highest score (4.69).
The technical quality of the photos is quite poor in the case GZ, and full-body pictures generally receive lower scores than close-up photos of the upper body only. Women also tend to give quite low points, if a man’s lower body is seen dressed so scantily as in this case. Thus there are three possible explanation, why the case GZ received much lower points than GA, although both questions include a quite similar male body (as one of the alternatives).
HA
1.89
upper body, front, shirt
buttons open, some of
chest and body hair seen
Pic 27, HA, 38 %. Pic 28, HB, 38 %. Pic 29, HC, 37 %.
The men in the case HA have their shirt buttons open, so that a similar view is formed deep down into the chest, as when a woman’s shirt has a very deep-cut neckline. Women did not appreciate such a style, and gave much lower points than in otherwise similar questions, in which the man’s shirt has a tight neckline (e.g. GB 4.59 points).
Homosexual men gave 5.00 points to men’s open neckline, with a sample of three respondents. This would have been the expected outcome also when the neckline of the shirt is ordinary and quite tight, so perhaps an open neckline did not have either a raising or a lowering impact on the score.
Hetero men gave very high scores for eroticness to women who had quite large breasts and a shirt with a very deep-cut neckline: as many as 6.97 points in the case AJ.
HB
1.88
lower body only, shorts
reach to 10 cm above
knee, front and behind
HC
1.76
skin-tight jeans, front
and behind, whole body,
muscular or ordinary
HD
1.59
3 cm long shorts,
sitting on a chair
with one knee up
HE
1.53
from waist to knees,
skin-tight underpants,
pouch around penis
HF
1.53
a man’s shins,
front and behind,
skin hair
HG
1.53
from waist to knees,
front and behind,
thongs cover very little
HH
1.44
upper body and pelvis,
the shape of penis shines
through swimpants
HI
1.39
kneeling on floor, some
crease of buttocks is seen
past low waist of jeans
HJ
1.28
in small underpants,
sitting or lying, crotch
is spread provocatively
HK
1.28
in skin-tight swimpants,
from waist to knees,
front and behind
These ten questions, which received quite low scores, indicate that heterosexual women have quite mild interest toward a man’s lower body in emphatically scanty clothing. Photos of women in equally scanty clothes receive very high scores for eroticness from heterosexual men.
Pic 30, HD, 32 %. Pic 31, HE, 31 %. Pic 32, HG, 31 %.
Pic 33, HH, 29 %. Pic 34, HI, 28 %. Pic 35, HJ, 26 %.
Case HE has unusual underpants to half-thigh, which have an additional pouch on the front side, which is designed to have quite precisely the shape of the penis. Such products are apparently sold in specialty shops of homosexuals only. Women were not attracted by such a homage to the male heirlooms, but homosexual men gave 7.00 points to this question, with a sample of two respondents.
Small thongs, which leave a man’s buttocks nearly completely bare, received 1.53 points from women in the case HG. Similar clothing with a wider view angle, so that the photo shows also the upper body, got only 0.76 points in case HW. Women dressed in similar very small bikinis received some 4.5 times higher scores from men (6.88) in the question AL.
Homosexual men gave surprisingly low points to these questions, 4.00 points in the case HW, and 1.00 in the case HG. In the case HV they gave 4.50 points to quite similar clothing. Thus nearly completely bare buttocks did not guarantee a very high score.
Question HH has men’s bodies from above knees to above navel, seen from front. The wet fabric of skin-tight swimpants has glued onto the skin, quite clearly revealing the three-dimensional shape of a penis that points diagonally upwards. Women did not get provoked by this view, and gave only 1.44 points for eroticness. Only one homosexual man answered this question, giving 4.00 points.
The logical counterpart for case HH in women’s clothing might be case AH, which got 7.06 points, as a vertical crease is formed at the crotch in a wet one-piece swimsuit, and the shape of nipples shines through the fabric of swimsuit.
HL
1.22
loose capri trousers
reach to middle of shins,
front and behind
Pic 36, HK, 26 %. Pic 37, HL*, 24 %. Pic 38, HM, 24 %.
* Picture 37 is not a photo that was used in the question HL, but rather a sample picture that has a similar style.
Women gave 2.94 points to men’s long loose trousers in the question GN, but 48 % lower points (1.53) to knee-length shorts in the case HF. A man’s trousers with capri length got even less style points from women (1.22), and maybe indeed based on the style aspect. Capri can be an esthetically divisive length of trousers, of which one cannot know whether they are too short trousers, or too long shorts.
HM
1.18
leggings from front or
rear or side, lower body,
muscular or athletic
The lower bodies of athletic or even muscular men in leggings excited women only little, when one considers the statistical success of muscularity, at least in photos that display the upper body. A homosexual man in the case GW received much higher points (2.18) with one photo only, though it had a better image quality than the somewhat blurry pictures in the question HM. Homosexual men gave 5.50 points to case HM.
HN
1.12
loose runner’s shorts
with some 10 cm long
hem, lower body only
Short runner’s shorts, which are familiar from the competitive sports of 1970’s, did not make a great impression on women, when worn by sporty men and seen from many directions. Homosexual men did not give more than 3.00 points either to this style of clothing. Perhaps there are esthetical reasons, why men’s shorts in basketball, football and at the beaches have become 20 – 30 cm longer during the past 50 years.
KE
1.11
loose shorts reach to 5 cm
above knee, with leggings
underneath, muscular
In the question HR close-up photos of a man’s shins in leggings received 0.83 points from women, and 1.67 points from homosexual men. Hetero men gave 3.58 points to close-up photos of women’s shins in leggings. Case KE has a wider view angle of the whole lower body, to which women gave 1.11 points, and homosexual men 1.00 points with a sample of one respondent only.
Pic 39, HN, 22 %. Pic 40, KE*, 22 %. Pic 41, HO, 20 %.
* Picture 40 is not a photo that was used in the question KE, but rather a sample picture that has a similar style.
HO
1.00
athletic, from behind,
tight shorts, bikini under-
pants shine through
Question HO is a male counterpart to case BT, in which men gave 4.75 points to the seams of underpants that are seen on the buttocks of women. Women did not have remarkable erotic interest in this detail, but one woman gave as many as 9 points. Homosexual men gave 3.00 points, with a sample of one respondent.
Pic 42, HP, 20 %. Pic 43, HQ, 20 %. Pic 44, HR, 17 %.
HP
1.00
skin-tight jeans, lower
body only, front, behind,
or sitting with legs spread
HQ
1.00
niqab-level clothing:
loose, head covered,
narrow strip of face seen
HR
0.83
close-up pictures
of men’s shins
in leggings
Question HQ included men’s clothing that covers the face with a scarf, which resembles islamic women’s niqab: a bedouin who protects himself against sand that is blown by the wind in the desert. Another photo had a snowboarder wearing winter clothes and protective sunglasses, which represented a quite similar level of clothing, but also wealth and stylishness. These aspects may have been in the mind of that woman, who gave full 10 points at this question. For eroticness? Or rather, for romanticness or adventurousness?
Homosexual men gave 1.50 points to case HQ. Hetero men gave 0.74 points to women wearing a niqab in the case CS.
Pic 45, HS, 16 %. Pic 46, HT, 16 %. Pic 47, HU, 15 %.
These six questions include very scanty clothing on men’s lower body. Some of these cases also have provocative body postures. Great popularity was not achieved by these means among heterosexual women: the average of scores given for eroticness is only 0.77 points for these six questions together.
HS
0.82
skin-tight wrestler’s suit
that reaches to half thigh,
on all fours, from the side
HT
0.78
shorts have 3 cm length,
front and behind, from
the waist to half shin
HU
0.76
in small swimpants,
on all fours, from side
and behind, kneeling
HV
0.76
pelvis and thighs behind,
wedge of the pants starts
at mid-height of buttocks
HW
0.76
from knees upwards,
front and behind,
thongs cover very little
HX
0.75
shorts have negative hem
length, 3 cm of buttocks
is left visible, behind
Pic 48, HV, 15 %. Pic 49, HW, 15 %. Pic 50, HX, 15 %.
The favourite of homosexual men among these six cases was HS, a wrestler in skin-tight clothing crawling on all fours on the carpet, which received as many as 6.33 points for eroticness. In the survey concerning women’s clothing a parallel to this is question AK, in which a woman in yoga suit crawls on the floor, seen from the side, which got 6.94 points for eroticness from heterosexual men.
When a sporty person crawls on all fours, seen from the side and dressed in skin-tight clothes, heterosexual men saw in the scenario eroticness worth 6.94 points, homosexual men worth 6.33 points, but hetero women only worth 0.82 points.
Question HX had tight shorts with a negative hem length, which left the lower edge of the buttocks visible at the height of 3 cm. Such clothing did not excite homosexual men, as the score was left as low as 1.67. Slightly poor quality of the photos might have a role in the low score, though. Similar shorts on women received 6.59 points from hetero men in case AS.
HY
0.72
men’s ankles,
feet and toes
in close-up photo
Men’s feet and toes did not inspire great emotions in women. Not in homosexual men either, who gave 1.00 points, with a sample of one respondent only. Hetero men appreciated women’s feet with nail polish on toes as worth 3.19 points in the case CG.
Pic 51, HY (0.72) 14 %. Pic 52, HZ (0.72) 14 %.
HZ
0.72
loose shorts reach
to half-thigh, with
leggings underneath
The same happened in the survey on men’s clothing as with women’s clothing, that shorts or a skirt reaching only to half-thigh received lower points for eroticness (HZ, 0.72) than shorts reaching to the knees over leggings (KE, 1.11). Technical quality of the photos may have a role in this. Does the picture 52 have poor lighting, or a distracting pattern on the fabric? It was generally an objective in this survey that clothes are single-coloured, without strong patterns, and not very dark (because a camera will not find as rich details from dark colour as the human eye sees in the real life), so that such details would not have a lowering effect on the given scores.
KA
0.71
upper body only seen
from behind, a lot
of acne on the back
Pic 53, KA (0.71) 14 %. Pic 22, GV (2.31) 46 %.
Young men’s backs with healthy skin received 2.31 points from hetero women in the question GV. A lot of acne on the backs of young men produced 69 % lower points. (These photos were not completely authentic: the acne was added in photo editing software, but perhaps the outcome is quite credible.)
Homosexual men gave 4.00 points to the healthy backs of young men, and 1.50 points to backs patterned by acne. Hetero men gave 5.87 points to women’s healthy backs in the case BD, and 3.16 points to backs with acne in the case CH. Slightly less acne had been added on women’s photos than on men’s photos.
These questions concerning acne, as well as question RA that concerns women’s removed breasts, offer a light scratch on the surface of the topic, what kind of an impact an outwardly visible sickness or disability has on a person’s attractiveness.
KB
0.65
man wearing leggings,
behind, standing, under-
pants shine through
Question KB had the same photo as the misleading case MB concerning women’s clothing. One can clearly detect through the fabric of the black leggings at the buttocks, that the man has white underpants, whose lower edge has a cut that starts at a very high level, which is typical for women only, and would leave 80 % of the buttocks bare, if the person did not have leggings on the underpants.. Underpants that leave so much of the buttocks bare are not even for sale marketed to men anywhere else than in specialty shops of homosexuals.
The popularity of this photo remained poor both among women (0.65) and homosexual men (2.00), although the man that is seen in the photo should be a potential object of sexual interest for precisely these groups of people. Only hetero men saw quite strong eroticness in this photo, worth 5.71 points (because they did not know that the person in the photo is a man).
KC
0.59
18-year-old men without
shirt, teenagelike youth-
ful slim upper body
Pic 54, KB, 13 %. Pic 55, KC, 12 %. Pic 56, KD, 11 %.
Question KC had shirtless 18 years old slim and teenagelike men, who did not have athletic muscles nor chest hair. The women who responded to the survey did not appreciate such an appearance of man, and gave to case KC the second-lowest points of all questions. Homosexual men gave points moderately, 4.50 with a sample of two respondents.
KD
0.56
upper body only, loose
shirt ends above navel,
some of stomach is seen
A so-called crop top or midriff shirt received from women the lowest score for eroticness as a style of a man’s clothing. Case GA, which got the highest score, has men without a shirt, and case GB that got second-highest points has men in a T-shirt. The logical expectation would be that a midriff shirt gets points at half-way between these two cases, because it is a compromise between a T-shirt and shirtlessness. In the questions concerning women’s clothing, men’s answers were as straightforward as this logically, but women’s answers concerning men’s clothing seem to be more complex what comes to their reasons and motives.
It is possible to find on the internet many surveys to women, in which stomach muscles have been voted to be even the most attractive part of a man’s body. Nevertheless, this attractive body part being seen from under a midriff shirt received a crushing review from women. This quite surprising outcome can be based on aspects of style: midriff shirt is not a typical or socially acceptable garment for a heterosexual man. The respondents may have interpreted that such clothing contains references to homosexuality.
Homosexual men gave 5.00 points for eroticness to this question, distributed into individual scores 9, 6 and 0. Hetero men gave 5.32 points to women wearing a belly shirt in the case BN, which is nearly 10 times more than women gave points to men wearing a belly shirt.
Man’s lower body & armpits: with or without skin hair?
SD
3.29
sleeveless shirt, one
arm raised, armpit
hair is shaven off
SE
1.47
sleeveless shirt, one
arm raised, armpit
hair is seen
Pic 57, SD (3.29) 66 %. Pic 58, SE (1.47) 29 %.
Questions SD and SE had otherwise very similar photos of men in a sleeveless shirt, one arm raised and the armpit visible, but in the case SD the men’s armpit hair was shaven off, and in the question SE the men had quite typical armpit hair in its natural length.
(In the best photo of question SD the man is more muscular than the men in the pictures of question SE. This might have raised the score of question SD, for other reasons than armpit hair. Unfortunately this was perceived only after making this survey, that it is necessary to pay so detailed attention to the features of the persons who are seen in the photos, for the questions to be comparable with each other,)
Men’s clean-shaven armpits received more than twice higher points for eroticness from women than armpit hair in its natural length. This is a noteworthy exception to the trend that skin hair in all parts of the body has generally guaranteed to men higher points from women than boyish lack of skin hair. In the case GA, which got the highest score from women (4.69 points), men had armpit hair in its full natural length, in addition to chest hair and stubble of beard, but the arms were in a natural postion downwards, so only very little of armpit hair was seen from behind the arms.
In the questions that concern woman’s clothing, the difference in the answers of hetero men was 4-fold between clean-shaven armpits (SA, 5.18 points) and armpit hair that is trimmed neatly to 1 cm length (SB, 1.21).
Homosexual men gave three times higher points to armpit hair (SE, 4.50) than clean-shaven armpits (SD, 1.50). They were the only group of people in this survey, who were more contented with armpits which have hair than which are hairless. The reliability of this finding is very poor, however, because among the three respondents the person who generally gave highest points in all questions, did not answer the question SD, and the person who generally gave the lowest points, did not answer the question SE.
HF
1.53
shins of man,
front and behind,
with skin hair
SF
1.06
shins of man, front
and behind, skin hair
completely shaven off
Men’s shins did not inspire great emotions in women, the score remained low. A man’s hairy shins were 50 % more erotic than clean-shaven shins in the opinion of women, however. Homosexual men saw more eroticness in men’s shins, but not much difference between these two alternatives: clean-shaven shins 3.50 points (SF), and shins with natural skin hair 3.00 points (HF), with a sample of two respondents.
Hetero men saw more eroticness in women’s shins, and it also mattered most to them, whether the shins have skin hair or not: clean-shaven shins got 4.63 points (BW), and women’s shins with dark skin hair 0.58 points only (SC).
Pic 59, HF (1.53) 31 %. Pic 60, SF* (1.06) 21 %.
* Picture 60 is not a photo that was used in the question SF, but rather a sample picture that has a similar style.
SG
1.19
3 cm shorts, front,
from waist to knees,
clean-shaven
HT
0.78
3 cm shorts, front and
behind, from waist to
half-shin, skin hair
Question SG had in a close-up photo a man’s pelvis and clean-shaven sporty thighs, in 3 cm long shorts (or actually underpants). Women gave 53 % more points for eroticness than in the case HT, in which there was skin hair on men’s thighs, and view angles also from behind. The photos of question HT had much poorer technical quality, so these two questions should perhaps not be compared with each other. The given scores are very low in any case, and the most popular score is 0 for both questions.
Homosexual men appreciated case SG to be worth as many as 6.33 points in eroticness, but case SG got only 1.67 points from them, with a sample of three respondents. Technical quality of the photos may emphasize this difference, but is probably not the only reason for such a remarkable difference between the scores.
Pic 61, SG (1.19) 24 %. Pic 62, HT (0.78) 16 %.
Man with or without pubic hair?
This survey did not ask about people’s opinion concerning pubic hair. Surveys on this topic have been done elsewhere, according to which over 80 % of women think that a man’s pubic hair is important esthetically. However, hairiness that grows with complete freedom is the favourite of few women only. Most women deem a version that is trimmed at a moderate length to be the most convenient option.
Man’s skin, tattooed or in its natural state?
TE
2.39
bare back,
large tattoos,
quite athletic
GE
4.29
upper body from behind,
muscular back, but
not a bodybuilder
GV
2.31
without shirt, from
behind only, bare
back, not muscular
This survey avoided using photos in which the person has tattoos in a very visible place. Parallel questions were made of a few styles of clothing, so that in one question the man does not have any tattoos, and in the other his skin is tattooed very extensively.
In the question TE the tattooed backs of sporty and only slightly muscular men received 2.39 points from women, which is approximately as much as they gave to men without tattoos who are not muscular (GV, 2.31). The backs of more clearly muscular men got much more points in the question GE (4.29).
Muscularity of the men in the case TE is perhaps half-way between cases GE and GV, so it remains unclear, whether tattoos had any influence or not on the scores given by women. At least not in a raising direction.
Pic 5, GE, 86 %. Pic 63, TE, 48 %. Pic 64, TF, 35 %.
Homosexual men gave 0 points to case TE, but the sample is one respondent only, so one should not draw any conclusions from this.
Pic 11, GM, 60 %. Pic 65, TG, 25 %. Pic 26, GZ, 39 %.
TF
1.76
upper body front,
no shirt, sporty,
many large tattoos
GM
3.00
upper body front,
no shirt, sporty,
clean-shaven skin
TG
1.25
whole body, front side,
swimpants, not muscu-
lar, many large tattoos
GZ
1.94
in swimpants, front, body
from the ankles upwards,
horizontal lower edge
These two pairs of questions compare the upper body of extensively tattooed men, seen from the front, to the closest similar question in which the man does not have tattoos. Perfect comparability of the questions cannot be guaranteed. Any possible comparison advantage in these cases, which might be based on differences in the quality of photos, was always in favour of the question in which the skin is not tattooed. But it is not probable that the technical quality of photos would fully explain these differences in the scores.
It is realistic to estimate that also women, similarly as heterosexual men, give approximately 25 % lower scores for eroticness to such persons, whose skin is tattooed very extensively. A small share of women give very high points also to abundantly tattooed men, however.
Homosexual men gave 4.50 points to the upper bodies of extensively tattooed men in the question TF, and 1.33 points to a tattooed full body in the case TG.
A question based on the use of imagination
NC
3.91
nude man, in a
quite neutral body
posture (imagined)
One question was based on using one’s imagination: a fully nude man. This question was accompanied by photos of very scantily dressed men, and cartoons or classical oil paintings of nude men. The respondents were asked to imagine, how erotic these men would be, if these had been photographs of naked real persons.
In the survey concerning women’s clothing, heterosexual men estimated the eroticness of a nude woman to be 7.16 points. This was assumed to be some 20 % below expectations (8.75), considering that men gave as many as 7.97 points to scantily dressed women, and that men’s enthusiasm to give high points was systematically the greater, the less the women had clothes on.
Hetero women estimated, based on imagination, that the eroticness of nude men is 3.91 points. This result may not be comparable with other questions, because of such a technical difference in how the question was presented. The outcome is also not straighforwardly logical, because nearly nude men received very low points from women. But a fully nude man would get quite high points from women, based on question NC.
A percentage is presented under the photos in this report, which is the share of the question’s points of the “highest possible” value, which is assumed to be 5.00 (which is a bit higher than the result 4.69 of question GA, which got highest points). It is not fully clear, however, how a man needs to be dressed in order to receive an average 5.00 points for eroticness from women.
It can be concluded from the diagram of question NC that the perception of heterosexual women of the eroticness of a nude man is distributed quite evenly on the full range from zero to ten points. A woman’s interest toward men’s nude bodies can have any level of mildness or strength, with nearly equally high statistical probability.
Homosexual men gave 5.67 points to a nude male body based on imagination. The sample was three respondents, and the answers were distributed 10, 7 and 0. The zero points given by one respondent is quite illogical, compared to the points that the same person has given in other questions, in which he has appreciated men’s scanty clothing often with quite high scores. Without this surprising zero-point answer the average would have been 8.50, which is of a similar level with the interest of heterosexual men toward the bodies of nude women (approximately 8.75).
Art pictures that portray a nude man
VE
3.67
cartoons drawn
in a quite realistic
style, nude man
VF
2.41
classical oil painting
from the years 1850
– 1939, nude man
VG
1.83
classical sculpture of
150 BCE – 1504 CE,
marble, nude man
VH
0.83
classical sculpture of
19th century, marble,
nude 11 years old boy
These art pictures portrayed fully nude men, created with the techniques cartoon, sculpture and classical oil painting. Modern cartoons containing nudity was regarded as quite erotic among hetero women, but the art of the classical period got very low points for eroticness, concentrating on zero points. A small share of women regarded all these artforms as strongly erotic.
Pic 66, VE, 73 %. Pic 67, VF, 48 %. Pic 68, VG, 37 %.
22 % of hetero women gave high points for eroticness (8 – 10 points) to cartoons drawn in a quite realistic style, most of which were japanese. 12 % of women deemed strongly erotic also oil paintings of the classical era. 6 % of women gave high points to classical sculpture art depicting men, and also to sculpture that portrays nude boys approximately 11 years of age.
Homosexual men gave 7.00 points to art paintings that portray nude men (sample 2 respondents), 6.00 points to realistic cartoons (1 respondent), 5.33 points to sculpture portraying nude men (3 respondents), and 3.00 points to sculpture that portrays nude boys approximately 11 years of age (3 respondents).
These questions used approximately the most erotic possible examples of art from the classical era, e.g. from the artists Konstantin Somov, Hiram Powers, and sculptors of ancient Greece and Rome who have remained anonymous. The answers do not represent typical feelings that nudity in classical art inspires in people, in the context of majority of artworks, but rather the potential peak values with each technique of classical art.
Pic 69, VH, 17 %. Pic 70, WA, 46 %. Pic 39, HN, 22 %.
Misleading pictures: man
WA
2.29
* woman, sprinter’s
very muscular legs,
3 cm long shorts
HN
1.12
loose runner’s shorts
with some 10 cm long
hem, lower body only
The survey included one case, in which the picture portrayed a woman, even though the assumed subject of the survey was the eroticness of a man’s clothing, The photo was cropped in such a way that the respondent had no means to conclude that the person in the photo is a woman and not a man. The only possible way would have been to recognize based on the shape of the body parts seen in the picture, that they belong to a woman and not a man.
Question WA contains the extremely muscular thighs and shins of a female sprint runner. Only the lower body is seen in the photo, from hips to ankles, front side. 17 heterosexual women answered this question, giving nearly twice higher points for eroticness than they gave to any parallel question that portrays a man’s lower body. But the scores involved here are relatively low anyway, 2.29 points only.
The (female) person depicted in the case WA was much more muscular than any of the photos that portrayed a man’s lower body, and the respondents had a general tendency to give higher scores to muscular persons, who are assumed to be men.
Homosexual men gave only 0.50 points to case WA, with a sample of two respondents, which is the second-lowest score among all 72 questions.
This same photo was used in the case CI of the survey concerning woman’s clothing, to which hetero men gave 3.03 points for eroticness.
Misleading pictures: boy child
YC
3.29
* upper body of 9 years
old boy, no face, muscles
are being flexed a bit
GA
4.69
no shirt, upper body from
front, beard, muscular, no
bodybuilder, chest hair
GM
3.00
upper body front,
no shirt, sporty,
clean-shaven skin
GO
2.89
upper body front, no
shirt, not muscular,
some chest hair
GP
2.78
upper body front,
no shirt, muscular,
clean-shaven skin
KC
0.59
18-year-old men without
shirt, teenagelike youth-
ful slim upper body
In two questions the photos portrayed a boy under the age of puberty, even though the assumed topic of the survey was eroticness of the clothing of adult men.
Case YC includes an approximately 9 years old boy whose hobby is boxing, who flexes his muscles a bit in the upper body and the arms. A white line is marked in picture 71 at chin level: the photo was shown cropped from this white line downwards, to make it more difficult for the respondents to estimate the age of the person that is seen in the photo.
Pic 1, GA, 94 %. Pic 71, YC, 66 %. Pic 11, GM, 60 %.
The survey included total six questions, which had a man’s upper body without shirt, seen from the front only. The approximately 9 years old boxer boy received the second-highest score (3.29) from women among these six questions, in the same middle caste with three other cases, which had adult men with an ordinary or muscular body.
Homosexual men gave 8.50 points for eroticness to case KC (with a sample of two respondents, who were the two persons who had a tendency to give highest scores generally). This is the highest score given by homosexual men among all 72 questions.
Pic 15, GO, 58 %. Pic 16, GP, 56 %. Pic 55, KC, 12 %.
YD
1.78
* 6 years old boy, swim-
pants, 5 cm hem, behind,
from waist to mid-thigh
HB
1.88
lower body only, shorts
reach to 10 cm above
knee, front and behind
HN
1.12
loose runner’s shorts
with some 10 cm long
hem, lower body only
A precise parallel for case YD is not found among the questions that portray adult men, because they nearly always had view angles from the front side too, and usually looser shorts. In the question YD the boy had skin-tight swimming pants.
Heterosexual women did not show much interest toward case YD, at the level of 1.78 points. Homosexual men gave 6.33 points, with a sample of three respondents, which is the 12th highest score that they gave among all 72 questions. This result is logically in line with the fact that they often gave high points to scanty or skin-tight clothing on a man’s lower body.
Pic 28, HB, 38 %. Pic 72, YD, 36 %. Pic 39, HN, 22 %.
We do not know, how old the respondents estimated the persons to be in questions YC and YD. Interest shown by adult men toward persons under the age of puberty is probably much less common statistically in the society than was the erotic interest indicated by the respondents toward limited details in the bodies of young children, when the age of the person was not known to the respondents.
With such experiment scenarios and by other means it is possible to try to find out, how adults make the decision not to show interest toward children’s bodies: Is a child’s body such by its shapes and features, that nothing in it spontaneously inspires erotic interest in adults? Or do some adults reject erotic triggers that they have detected, either on the level of understanding or subconsciously, as they become aware that the context of the potentially erotic trigger is reproachable morally or philosophically?
Photos:2. unsplash.com / Mitchell Luo, 3. unsplash.com / Jakob Owens, 4. pexels.com / The Lazy Artist Gallery, 5. pixabay.com / Valentin Tixonov, 6. pexels.com / Shalender Kumar, 7. pexels.com / Jordan Bergendahl, 8. unsplash.com / Hermes Rivera, 9. pexels.com / Tima Miroshnichenko, 12. freepik.com / Halayalex, 13. pixabay.com / Epicioci Mayor, 14. photographer who is anonymous but known to the author of this book, 17. photo-ac.com / acworks, 20. pexels.com / Rodnae Productions, 21. pexels.com / Ali Atiabii, 22. unsplash.com / Christopher Campbell, 24. unsplash.com / Gabin Vallet, 27. unsplash.com / Christian Buehner, 37. pexels.com / Cottonbro Studio, 40. pexels.com / Pavel Danilyuk, 43. unsplash.com / Karsten Winegeart, 44. unsplash.com / Chris Moore, 51. photo-ac.com / acworks, 52. pexels.com / Pavel Danilyuk, 53. unsplash.com / Adrian Rosco, 56. freepik.com / freepik, 57. unsplash.com / Omid Armin, 58. pexels.com / Alex Barnes, 59. photo-ac.com / w****************m, 60. pexels.com / Tima Miroshnichenko, 63. unsplash.com / Norbert Buduczki, 64. pexels.com / Cottonbro Studio.
Q09B Answers as a data table
The given answers are indicated in these data tables added by one point: for example, 1 means that the respondent gave the score 0. Empty means that the question was not answered at all.
Answers by homosexual men (3)
Questions 1/4 NC – GB:
id NC YD YC VE VF VG VH HQ GH GD HC GC TG GZ GY HV HW GL GF GG GX GB

 7  8  4  8  7  7  7  5  1  7  9  5  3  1  6  3  2  3  8  4  5  4  5
13 11  9 11     9  6  6  4  6           5 11     9 11  9          10
30  1  9           6  1     5     6     1     3     1  1  1
Questions 2/4 GK – GW:
id GK GT SD SE GE KA TE KD GU HA GP GO KC TF HY GN GQ HP HN HZ HM GW

 7  4  4  4  3  4  2  1  7  7  5  2  3  5  1  2  7  8 10  2  2  8  4
13           8 10  3    10 10  7    11  6 10     6     4        5
30  1     1              1     6     1                             9
Questions 3/4 GJ – HI:
id GJ HL SF HF HR HB GR HN HT WA HX HK GM HG HS HU HJ HD HE GS HH HI

 7  7  4  4  3  2  4  3  3  2  2  2  4  4  3  3  3  3  4  6  9  5  3
13        5  5  5  7     5  5     5          10  7 11    10
30              1     1     1  1  1        1  9     3  1
Questions 4/4 HO – KB:
id HO GA SG GV GI KB

 7  4  3  2  4  2  2
13    11 11 10 11  6
30     6  9  1  1  1
Answers by heterosexual women (24)
Questions 1/4 NC – GB:
id NC YD YC VE VF VG VH HQ GH GD HC GC TG GZ GY HV HW GL GF GG GX GB

 2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  4  1  4  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  4
 3  3  2  4 11 11  4  1  1  5  5  2  7  5  3  1  2  2  2  7  6  3  2
 4  1  1  4  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  2  3  2  1  1  1  3  6  1  2  6
 6  5  6           4  4     4                          3
 8        1              1                          1
 9 11  9  8 10 10 10 11  1  5  9  7  9  6  9  9 10 10  4  5  5  8  4
10  8        1  7              8           5     3           8  7  8
11  6  7  6  2  1  1  1  1  7  4  5  4  2  4  4  1  2  7  5  9  6  8
12 10                             7 10       11           2
14  6  3  3  7  1  1  1  1  3  7  4 10  1  1  1  1  1  1  5  7  6  5
15  2  1 10  3  1  2  1  1  3  9  1  9  1  1  6  1  1  9  1  9  1  8
16  6  3  1  9  7  4  1  1 11 11  1  3  1  4  1  1  1  2 10  9  1 10
17 11       11                 8    11                   10  3  3
18  1  1  7  1  1  1  1 11  8  4  4  1  3  1  1  1  1  5  5  5  1  5
19  9  6           7  1     9                         10
20  1  1  3     1  1  1  4  5     1     1  1  1  1  1  6           5
21  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  2  2
22
23  2  2  5  2  3  3  1  2  8  4  3  7  5  6  3  1  1  5  8  6  3  8
24  8  3  8  5  7  6  3  4  6  5  4  8  3  7  4  2  3  8  8  8  5  8
25  1  1  6  1  1  1  1  1  3  2  1  5  1  1  1  1  1  3  4  3  2  6
26  3  1  2  6  3  1  1  1  4  3  3  5  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  4  4  2
27  5        6                 9     1                       5  1
29  7  1  3  6  1  2  1  1  5  3  1  2  1  2  3  1  1  3  6  2  1  4
Questions 2/4 GK – GW:
id GK GT SD SE GE KA TE KD GU HA GP GO KC TF HY GN GQ HP HN HZ HM GW

 2  1  1  4  1  1  1  6  1  3  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
 3  3  4  3  6  9  4  7  2  5  4  7  4  1  7  1  5  2  2  1  1  2  1
 4  3  2  1  1  3  1  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  4  1  3  1  2  1  1  2  1
 6                             4     2
 8                       1                             1
 9  6  7  8  2 10  2  6  6  3  8 10  7 10  2  4  3 10 10  6  5 10 10
10     7     7  6  1  9     9     5     1 10  3  6  8     3  4  5
11  6  3  5  2  8  1  7  3  6  5  7  9  1  5  3  3  1  1  3  1  2  5
12  2     8                                                       11
14  7  9  6  1  4  1  3  1  1  1  8  2  1  1  1  1  7  2  2  1  3  2
15  2  1  1  1  4  1  1  1  3  1  2  2  1  1  1  4  1  1  1  1  1  1
16 11  2  9  6  9  3  1  1  9  4  4  7  1  1  2 11 11  3  1  1  1  1
17  8  8 11           3           1           1     8     2  3     8
18  3  1  1  1  1  1  5  1  1  1  1  1  1  4  1  5  1  1  1  1  1  1
19                       1     6     9
20              4  4     1  4  3     3  1  1     5     1        1
21  1  1  1  1  3  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
22           1                                            1
23  7  1  2  4  5  1  1  1  1  5  1  9  1  1  1  5  2  1  3  3  2  3
24  5  6  4  5  8  4  4  3  3  3  7  8  2  4  6  5  4  3  8  3  2  4
25  4  4  3  1  5  1  1  1  6  1  3  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  1  1
26  2  1  5  1  6  1  1  1  1  2  6  2  1  2  1  2  1  2  1  1  1  2
27     4              1           1           1     5     1  1
29  2  1  1  1  4  1  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  6  1  1  1  1  1  1
Questions 3/4 GJ – HI:
id GJ HL SF HF HR HB GR HN HT WA HX HK GM HG HS HU HJ HD HE GS HH HI

 2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  4
 3  9  2  1  6  1  4  4  3  1  7  1  2  3  3  2  3  4  2  9  8  8  1
 4  1  1  4  1  1  2  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  5  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
 6              2           2                       6
 8           1                                1
 9 10  2  3  2  7  2  8  3  8  9 10  9  6 10  6  5 11  9 10 10 11  9
10  9  4  8        6     6           3  7        4        4  6  4  5
11  4  1  1  6  1  2  6  7  3  8  1  3  3  2  2  1  2  3  2  1  1  4
12                    9       11          11          11
14  7  1  4  1  1  2  3  1  1  1  1  4 10  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
15  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  7  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
16  3  4  1  2  1  6  3  3  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  2  1  1 11  1  1
17 11  4                             1 11  1           1     5  1  1
18  1  1  3  1  1  4  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
19              3           3                       2
20        1  2  1     3  1  1  1  1           1  1  1     1
21  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  1  2  1  1  1  1  1  1  2  1  1
22                 6                    3                          1
23  5  6  1  4  2  3  4  1  1  3  1  6  3  2  1  1  1  1  2  1  1  2
24  6  7  1  8  3  3  4  3  3  3  4  3  3  4  3  4  3  7  5  7  7  5
25  1  1  1  1  1  4  3  1  1  1  1  1  6  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1
26  5  1  2  1  2  1  3  1  1  4  1  1  3  1  2  1  1  1  1  3  1  3
27  6  1                             1                       1  1
29  1  1  1  4  3  1  5  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  3  1  1
Questions 4/4 HO – KB:
id HO GA SG GV GI KB

 2  1  4  1  1  1  1
 3  2  9  2  5  9  2
 4  1  1  1  1  1  1
 6
 8
 9 10  9 10  8  8  8
10  6
11  1  9  1  4  9  1
12
14  1  4  3  2  9  1
15  1  6  1  5  2  1
16  1 11  1  4 10  1
17  1
18  1  1  2  1  1  1
19                 1
20     3  3  3  5  1
21  1  2  1  1  2  1
22
23  2  8  1  7  3  1
24  3  9  4  4  8  4
25  1  5  1  4  4  1
26  1  4  2  2  2  1
27  1
29  1  6  1  1  2  1
                                     
Copyright © 2023–2025 Ion Mittler. All rights reserved.